期刊文献+

青少年聋生的分类学联系 被引量:16

Deaf Adolescents' Representations of Taxonomic Relations
下载PDF
导出
摘要 为了考察青少年聋生分类学联系的特点,使用不同类别的基本水平概念,完成了语义归类和错误再认2个实验,并同健听生的结果进行比较。结果表明:⑴聋生和健听生有类似的类别意识,但聋生的类别意识较健听生为弱。⑵与健听生比,呈现方式对聋生的反应影响较小。聋生的分类学概念有较强的形象化倾向。青少年聋生分类学联系的特点与他们语言能力发展迟缓、使用自然手语有关。因此,应加强聋生的规范手语教学,重视培养聋生的抽象思维能力。 Because deaf people differs from hearing people in so many ways, such as using a different language and having a relatively isolated way of living a reasonable hypothesis is that their representation of world knowledge might have some unique characteristics compared to hearing people. In the current study we sought to investigate their representation of taxonomic categories. Our hypothesis was that deaf adolescents had stronger tendency of imagery representation of exemplars and weaker awareness of taxonomic categories than their intelligently-matched hearing counterparts. In experiment 1, 15 deaf adolescents and 15 intelligently-matched hearing counterparts (31 months younger) were recruited to complete a semantic categorization task. There were two groups of experimental stimuli. One was hightypicality group composed of 16 taxonomic categories, in which 3 exemplars of higher typicality and 1 of lower typicality were selected as prime and target. The other was low-typicality group again of 16 taxonomic categories, in which 3 exemplars of lower typicality and 1 of higher typicality were selected as prime and target. All the exemplars from 25 taxonomic categories were presented in forms of words and pictures and subjects' reaction time and accuracy were recorded. In experiment 2, 16 deaf adolescents and 16 intelligently-matched hearing counterparts (38 months younger) were recruited to complete a false memory task. There were two kinds of experimental stimuli. The first group was composed of exemplars from 13 categories, in which 3 exemplars of high typicality and 1 of low typicality were used as study item and lure. The second group was composed of exemplars from another 13 categories, in which 3 exemplars of low typicality and 1 of high typicality used as study item and lure. All the exemplars were presented in forms of words and pictures and the purpose was to further test our hypothesis. The results showed for experiment I, both deaf and hearing adolescents responded faster to the stimuli sets in which the primes were exemplars of higher typicality than lower typicality. But deaf adolescents responded slower to picture stimuli than words while hearing adolescents' reaction time remained the same. Besides, deaf subjects made more errors when the primes were exemplars of higher typicality. Similar results were obtained from Experiment 2. Deaf adolescents had lower recognition accuracy when target items were of lower typicality while hearing subjects' recoguition accuracy was not influenced by exemplar typicality. For the recognition accuracy of lures, deaf subjects made more error than hearing subjects. Furthermore the hearing subjects' recognition was influenced by exemplars typicality and item presentation mode while the deaf subjects were not. To sum up, the current study demonstrated that deaf and hearing adolescents had similar typicality awareness. But due to their persistent dependence on the use of sign language, deaf adolescents had weaker awareness of a taxonomic category than their hearing counterparts. Instead, they had a stronger tendency to use imagery representation of taxonomic categories than their hearing counterparts.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2008年第11期1178-1189,共12页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 国家重点基础研究发展计划"973"课题(2005CB522802) 全国教育科学"十一五"规划重点课题(DBA080164) 广东省普通高校人文社会科学重点研究基地重大研究项目(06JDXMXLX01) 广东省自然科学基金团队项目(06200524)资助
关键词 青少年聋生 分类学概念 分类学联系 deaf adolescents taxonomic concept taxonomic relation
  • 相关文献

参考文献57

  • 1Rosch E H. Cognitive representation of semantic categories.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1975, 104:192 -233
  • 2Murphy G L. Causes of taxonomic sorting by adults: A test of the thematic-to-taxonomic shift. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2001, 8 (4): 834 -839
  • 3Stokoe W C Jr. Sign language structure: An outline of the visual communication systems of the American deaf. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2005, 10 (1): 3 -37
  • 4Campbell R, Wright H. Deafness and immediate memory for pictures: dissociations between "inner speech" and the "inner ear"? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990, 50:259-286
  • 5Koh S D, Vernon M, Bailey W. Free-recall learning of word lists by pre-lingual deaf subjects. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1971, 10:542-547
  • 6Osberger M, Hesketh L. Speech and language disorders related to hearing impairment. In: Lass N, Northern J, et al (Eds.) Handbook of speech-Language pathology and audiology. Toronto: BC Decke, 1986. 858 -886
  • 7Jerger S, Lai L, Marchman V A. Picture naming by children with hearing loss: I. Effect of semantically related auditory distracters. Journal of American Academic Audiology, 2002, 13:463 -477
  • 8Zarfaty Y, Nunes T, Bryant P. The performance of young deaf children in spatial and temporal number tasks. Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2004, 9 (3): 315 - 326
  • 9Bull R, Marschark M, Blatto-Vallee G. SNARC hunting: Examining number representation in deaf students. Learning and Individual Differences, 2005, 15:223 -236
  • 10Rettenback R, Diller G, Sireteaunu R. Do deaf people see better? Texture segmentation and visual search compensate in adult but not in juvenile subjects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 1999, 11 : 560 - 583

二级参考文献133

共引文献252

同被引文献283

引证文献16

二级引证文献70

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部