期刊文献+

负数的空间表征机制 被引量:24

The Mechanism of Negative Numbers' Spatial Representation
下载PDF
导出
摘要 本研究采用快速数字大小分类范式,每次试验呈现一个数字,要求被试快速判断即时呈现的数字大于或小于-5(或5),探讨负数在心理数字线上的表征方向问题。实验一将负数(-1~-9)和正数(1~9)分两组分别呈现;实验二将正负数混合呈现,仅对负数进行反应。结果表明,负数按照其绝对值大小表征在心理数字线上,绝对值小的负数表征在心理数字线的左侧,绝对值大的负数表征在心理数字线的右侧。该结果支持系统进化论假说。 The spatial representation of numbers has been intensively investigated in the field of number processing mechanism. To better explore the spatial representation, the mental number line hypothesis has been proposed, which gets support from research on distance effect and SNARC effect. Although much has been done to investigate the spatial representation of positive numbers, studies focused on negative numbers are rather scarce. Moreover, the results on negative numbers are essentially inconsistent, in that there are two major opposite hypotheses on the spatial representation: the phylogenetic hypothesis and the ontogenetic hypothesis. The phylogenetic hypothesis suggests that negative numbers are mapped onto the mental number line according to their absolute numerical value: the small absolute value on the left, and the large absolute value on the right. Conversely, the ontogenetic hypothesis asserts that negative numbers are mapped onto the mental number line according to the numerical value: the small value on the left, and the large value on the right. Further analysis of the two hypotheses reveals that two different paradigms were adopted in previous research, namely the odd-even judgment paradigm and the magnitude judgment paradigm, which might be explicable for the contrasting conclusions. In addition, both paradigms are essentially improper for the research on the representation of negative numbers. Considering the two existing problems, this study adopted the speeded magnitude classification paradigm to further investigate the spatial representation of negative number processing. Two experiments were conducted with the numbers ranging from-9 to 9, but excluding 0 as materials. On each trial, a rectangle-frame was firstly displayed in the center of the screen as fixation, then a number was displayed horizontally in the center of fixation. The participant should classify the magnitude as quickly and accurately as possible. The first experiment, which contained half of positive numbers judgment and half of negative numbers judgment, was aimed at investigating the difference between two types of number processing and the direction of spatial representation. The second experiment was aimed at further investigating the direction of negative numbers spatial representation through a mixed display method, in which both positive numbers and negative numbers were displayed serially but only negative numbers were targets of magnitude judgment. We varied the consistency between the response key and digit magnitude as the independent variable in Experiment 1 and 2. Each experiment included eighteen participants. ANOVA analyses and regression analyses were performed in both experiments. The data analyses showed that SNARC effect appeared in both experiments. Specifically, participants responded faster as to the left-side decision for negative numbers with small absolute value and faster as to the right-side decisions for negative numbers with large absolute value. The pattern of decision latencies suggested that negative numbers are mapped onto the mental number line according to their absolute value instead of numerical value, thus supporting the phylogenetic hypothesis.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2009年第2期95-102,共8页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 教育部高等学校博士学科点专项科研基金(20060335034) 教育部哲学社会科学研究重大课题攻关项目(07JZD0029) 国家自然科学基金(30870765 30570604) 国家基础科学人才培养基金(J0630760)
关键词 负数的空间表征 心理数字线 SNARC效应 系统进化论假说 个体发展论假说 negative number mental number line SNARC effect phylogenetic hypothesis ontogenetic hypothesis
  • 相关文献

参考文献32

  • 1Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J., &Ryan, P. M. ( 1999 ). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals : developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74 , 286 - 308.
  • 2Calabria, M. , & Rossetti, Y. (2005). Interference between number processing and line bisection: a methodology. Neuropsychologia, 43,779 -783.
  • 3Campbell, J. I. (1999). The surface form x problem size interaction in cognitive arithmetic: evidence against an encoding locus. Cognition, 70,25 - 33.
  • 4Casarotti, M. , Michielin, M. , Zorzi, M. , &Umilita, C. (2007). Temporal order judgment reveals how number magnitude affects visuospafial attention. Cognition, 102,101 - 117.
  • 5Castronovo, J., & Seron, X. (2007). Semantic numerical representation in blind subjects : The role of vision in the spatial format of the mental number line. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60,101 - 119.
  • 6Dehaene, S. (2003). The neural basis of the Weber-Fechner law: a logarithmic mental number line. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 145 - 147.
  • 7Dehaene, S., Bossini. S., &Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and numerical magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,122,371 -396.
  • 8Dehaene, S., Dupoux, E., &Mehler, J. (1990). Is numerical comparison digital? Analogical and symbolic effects in two-digit number comparison. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16,26 - 641.
  • 9Fias, W. , & Verguts, T. (2004). The mental number line : exact and approximate. Trends in Cognitive Scienses, 8,447 -448.
  • 10Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., &Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,8,307 -314.

二级参考文献36

  • 1[1]Wynn K. Psychological foundations of number : numerical competence in human infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998, 2(8): 296~303
  • 2[2]Koechlin E, Dehaene S, Mehler J. Numerical transformations in five-month-old human infants . Math. Cognition,1998, 3: 89~104
  • 3[3]Dehaene S, Dehaene-Lambertz G, Cohen L. Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain .Trends Neurosci, 1998, 21: 355~361
  • 4[4]Feigenson L, Carey S, Spelke E S. Infants' discrimination of number vs. continuous extent.Cognitive Psychology, 2002, 44: 33~66
  • 5[5]Simon, T, Hespos, S, Rochat, P. Do infants understand simple arithmetic? A replication of Wynn(1992). Cognitive Development, 1995, 10: 253~269
  • 6[6]Moore D, Benenson J, Reznick J S, Peterson M. Effect of auditory numerical information on infants' looking behavior: contradictory evidence. Developmental Psychology, 1987, 23(5): 665~670
  • 7[7]Wynn K. Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 1992, 358: 749~750
  • 8[8]Uller C, Carey S, Huntley-Fenner G, et al. What representations might underlie infant numberical knowledge?.Cognitive Development, 1997, 14: 1~36
  • 9[9]Meck W H, Church R M. A mode control model of counting and timing processes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 1983, 9: 320~334
  • 10[10]Simon T J. Reconceptualizing the origins of numerical knowledge: a 'non-numerical' account. Cognition. 1997, 12: 349~372

共引文献13

同被引文献301

引证文献24

二级引证文献41

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部