期刊文献+

结果的接近性对正性结果评价影响的电生理证据 被引量:1

Positive Outcome Evaluation Is Modulated by Closeness of Outcome:An ERP Research
下载PDF
导出
摘要 结果的接近性是指对已发生事件的实际结果与在心理上的理想结果或期待结果之间差距的接近程度。实验采用事件相关电位技术和类老虎机赌博任务探讨了结果的接近性对正性结果评价的影响。(1)行为评定结果表明,结果的接近性不同水平诱发了不同的情绪体验,"差一点输"的赢钱使被试更多感到庆幸,"最理想"的赢钱使被试感到高兴;结果的接近性也影响了随后trial的反应时,与"一般"的赢钱和"最理想"的赢钱相比,"差一点输的"赢钱后的下一个trial中被试的反应时明显变长。(2)脑电结果表明,结果的接近性不同水平诱发了不同的FRN和P300,与"一般"的赢钱和"最理想"的赢钱相比,"差一点输"的赢钱诱发了波幅更大的FRN和波幅更大潜伏期更长的P300。这说明结果的接近性明显地影响了正性结果的认知评价加工。 Closeness of outcome refers to the degree of distance between real behavioral outcomes and desired outcomes. Previous studies revealed that closeness of outcome was one of the main factors that induce counterfactual thinking about the outcome, and played an important role in inducing post-decisional emotions (e.g., regret, rejoicing). Although some recent researches have explored how closeness of outcome affects negative outcome evaluation (e.g., losing money) in gambles, there is no evidence that whether it modulates positive outcome evaluation (e.g., gaining money). By simultaneously recording event-related potentials (ERPs) data from participants engaged in a simple like-slot-machine gambling task, the present study aimed to further investigate that whether and how closeness of outcome modulated feedback outcome evaluation when gaining money. Fourteen healthy undergraduates (7 males and 7 females, age 19-24, M=21.6 years) were recruited to take part to this study. The participants in the experiment were all right-hand, had normal or corrected-to-normal vision and had no neurological or psychological disorders. Each participant signed a consent form prior to the experiments and was paid after the experiment. The study was approved by the local academic committee. In this experimental task, closeness of feedback outcome was manipulated through depending on the distance between feedback outcome and desired outcome in the screen in the gambling task. Thus, three conditions were included in analyzed results: near-gain, general-gain, and full-gain. The EEG was recorded from 64 scalp channels using electrodes mounted in an elastic cap. Feedback-related ERPs, including feedback-related negativity (FRN) and P300, were calculated for a 1200 ms epoch including a 200 ms pre-feedback baseline. The amplitude of FRN and P300 were peek-to-peak measured separately in the 200-300 ms and 300-500 ms time windows after the appearance of feedback. Behavioral results suggested that emotional response of the participants was obviously modulated by the closeness of outcome. Specially, participants tended to use the emotional word "rejoicing" to describe their feelings when near-gains, and tended to use the word "elation" to describe their feelings when full-gains. In addition, the closeness of feedback outcome also modulated the reaction time of participants in the next trial following it, in which, participants' reaction time was evidently longer when near-gains, compared with the one when full-gains and general-gains. ERP results indicated that the FRN and P300 were sensitive to the closeness of outcome, with near-gains eliciting larger amplitudes of the FRN and larger amplitudes and longer latencies of the P300, compared with full-gains and general-gains. These results revealed that positive outcome evaluation is also modulated obviously by closeness of outcome.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2012年第8期1047-1057,共11页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 国家自然科学基金项目(81171289) 中央高校基本科研业务经费专项资金(SWU1109009) 西南大学国家重点学科基础心理学211工程项目(NSKD11025) 西南大学研究生科技创新基金优博项目(KY2009013)资助
关键词 结果的接近性 结果评价 FRN P300 ERP closeness of outcome outcome evaluation FRN P300 ERP
  • 相关文献

参考文献6

二级参考文献149

  • 1徐晓坤,王玲玲,钱星,王晶晶,周晓林.社会情绪的神经基础[J].心理科学进展,2005,13(4):517-524. 被引量:15
  • 2卿素兰,罗杰.大学生后悔心理调查[J].中国心理卫生杂志,2005,19(12):810-810. 被引量:8
  • 3陈俊,贺晓玲,张积家.反事实思维两大理论:范例说和目标-指向说[J].心理科学进展,2007,15(3):416-422. 被引量:31
  • 4Anderson, A. K., Wais, P. E., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2006). Emotion enhances remembrance of neutral events past. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103, 1599-1604.
  • 5Bechara, A., Tranel, D. & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123, 2189-2202.
  • 6Bell, D. E. (1982). Regret in decision making under certainty. Operations Research, 30, 961-981.
  • 7Betas, G.. S., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G, & Montague, P. R. (2001). Predictability modulates human brain response to reward, Journal of Neuroscience, 21, 2793-2798.
  • 8Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulated cortex: an update. Trends in Cognitive Science, 8, 539-546.
  • 9Brcitcr, H. C., Aharon, I., Kahncman, D., Dale, A., & Shizgal, P. (2001). Functional imaging of neural responses to expectancy and experience of monetary gains and losses. Neuron, 30, 619-639.
  • 10Byrne, R. M. (2002). Mental models and counterfactual thoughts about what might have been. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 426-431.

共引文献57

同被引文献36

  • 1刘 超,买晓琴,傅小兰.不同注意条件下的空间-数字反应编码联合效应[J].心理学报,2004,36(6):671-680. 被引量:42
  • 2Aryee, S., Chen, Z. X., Sun, L. Y., & Debrah, Y. A. (2007) . An- tecedents and outcomes of abusive supervision: Test o a trick- le-down model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92 (1) , 191.
  • 3Azizian, A., & Polich, J. (2007) . Evidence for attentional gradient in the serial position memory curve from event-related poten- tials. Journal of cognitive neuroscience, 19 (12) , 2071-2081.
  • 4Byrne, A., Dionisi, A. M., Barling, J., Akers, A., Robertson, J., Lys, R., & Dupr6, K. (2014) . The depleted leader: The influence of leaders' diminished psychological resources on leadership behaviors. The Leadership Quarterly, 25 (2) , 344-357.
  • 5Chi, S. C. S., & Liang, S. G. (2013) tion-regulation strategies matter? nares' emotional exhaustion, and When do subordinates' emo- Abusive work withdrawal. The Leader-ship Quarterly, 24 (1): 125-137.
  • 6Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (1997) . Cerebral pathways for calcula- tion: Double dissociation between rote verbal and quantitative knowledge of arithmetic. Cortex, 33 (2) , 219-250.
  • 7Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Cohen, L. (1998) . Abstract representations of numbers in the anima| and human brain. Trends in neurosciences, 21 (8) , 355-361.
  • 8Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2004) . Interaction between the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system predicts better memory for emotional events. Neuron, 42 (5) , 855-863.
  • 9Garcia, P. R. J. M., Wang, L., Lu, V., Kiazad, K., & Restubog, S. L. D. (2015) . When victims become culprits: The role of sub- ordinates' neuroticism in the relationship between abusive su- pervision and workplace deviance. Personality and Individual Differences, 72, 225-229.
  • 10Gehring, W. J., & Willoughby, A. R. (2002) . The medial frontal cortex and the rapid processing of monetary gains and losses. Science, 295 (5563) , 2279-2282.

引证文献1

二级引证文献2

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部