期刊文献+

愤怒的动机方向 被引量:15

The Motivational Direction of Anger
下载PDF
导出
摘要 动机是动物行为的基本推动力。愤怒作为一种特殊的负性情绪通常具有趋近动机的性质,但在某些情境下却跟回避动机有关。对这些冲突的证据有两种解释,其一认为愤怒的特异性成分可能与趋近动机有关,而其非特异性成分与回避动机有关;其二认为,愤怒外投与趋近动机有关,愤怒内投与回避动机有关。大量研究暗示了愤怒与趋近动机的关联具有优先性与一般性,而愤怒与回避动机的关联则是有条件的,受情境制约的。将来需要进一步探索愤怒与趋近动机的进化和认知神经机制,并明确愤怒与回避动机相关联的情境特征。 Animal behavior is driven by motivation essentially. Anger, as a special negative emotion, was thought to be related to the approach motivational system. Several studies, however, found that anger was associated with the avoidance motivational system. Some scholars explained these conflict findings by suggesting that the nonspecific component of anger was related to avoidance motivation, whereas its specific component had a stronger link to approach motivation. While some others linked approach motivation with anger-out, and avoidance motivation with anger-in. However, considerable data implied the association between anger and approach motivation were preferred and common, and the association between anger and avoidance motivation was conditioned and limited by the situation. More studies are needed to uncover the evolutionary anger-approach motivation link as well as its cognitive and neural mechanism, and to clarify the situational characters of anger-avoidance motivation link.
作者 杜蕾
机构地区 德州学院教育系
出处 《心理科学进展》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2012年第11期1843-1849,共7页 Advances in Psychological Science
关键词 愤怒 动机 趋近 回避 anger motivation approach avoidance
  • 相关文献

参考文献45

  • 1邹吉林,张小聪,张环,于靓,周仁来.超越效价和唤醒——情绪的动机维度模型述评[J].心理科学进展,2011,19(9):1339-1346. 被引量:67
  • 2Albert, D., Jonik, R., & Walsh, M. (1992). Hormonedependent aggression in male and female rats: Experiential, hormonal, and neural foundations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 16(2), 177-192.
  • 3Balconi, M., & Mazza, G. (2010). Lateralisation effect in comprehension of emotional facial expression: A comparison between EEG alpha band power and behavioural inhibition (BIS) and activation (BAS) systems Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 15(3), 361-384.
  • 4Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Emotion and motivation. In 1. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson, (Eds.), Handbook ofpsychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 581-607). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • 5Brookshire, G., & Casasanto, D. (2011). Motivation andmotor control: Hemispheric specialization for motivation reverses with handedness. In L. Carlson, C. Hoilscher & T. Shipley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 33rd annual conference of the cognitive science society (pp. 2610-2615). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • 6Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183-204.
  • 7Colibazzi, T., Posner, J., Wang, Z., Gorman, D., Gerber, A,, Yu, S., et al. (2010). Neural systems subserving valence and arousal during the experience of induced emotions. Emotion, 10(3), 377-389.
  • 8Davidson, R. J., & Irwin, W. (1999). The functional neuroanatomy of emotion and affective style. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3(1), 11-21.
  • 9Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In T. Dalgleish & M. J. Power (Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 45 -60). Chichester, U.K: Wiley.
  • 10Elliot, A. J. (2006). The Hierarchical model of approach avoidance motivation. Motivation and Emotion, 30(2), 111 -116.

二级参考文献37

  • 1Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 581-607). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • 2Briggs, K. E., & Martin, F. H. (2009). Affective picture processing and motivational relevance: Arousal and valence effects on ERPs in an oddball task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72, 299-306.
  • 3Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 183-204.
  • 4Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions Revealed. New York: Times Books.
  • 5Franken, I. H. A., Van Strien, J. W., Bocanegra, B. R., & Huijding, J. (2011). The P3 event-related potential as an index of motivational relevance: A conditioning experiment. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25, 32-39.
  • 6Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218-226.
  • 7Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-332.
  • 8Gable, E A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008a). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19, 476-482.
  • 9Gable, E A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008b). Relative left frontal activation to appetitive stimuli: Considering the role of individual differences. Psychophysiology, 45, 275-278.
  • 10Gable, P. A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2010a). The motivational dimensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorization. Emotion and Cognition, 24, 322-337.

共引文献66

同被引文献315

引证文献15

二级引证文献59

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部