期刊文献+

视觉工作记忆负载对听觉偏差干扰效应的调控:来自不同外周提示线索的证据 被引量:4

The Modulation of Auditory-visual Deviant Distraction by Working Memory Load: Evidence from Periphery Visual Cue with Different Validity
下载PDF
导出
摘要 采用线索提示和视听Oddball结合的实验设计,通过四个实验探讨了外周视线索下视觉工作记忆负载对听觉偏差干扰效应的调控作用。结果表明:(1)操作外周视线索有效性为50%的条件下,视觉工作记忆负载的增加使得原本抑制无关声音的过程失败,因此出现了显著的听觉偏差干扰效应;(2)操作外周视线索有效性为80%的条件下,发现有策略性的注意定向消耗了注意资源,无视觉工作记忆负载的情况下同样出现听觉偏差干扰,并且随着工作记忆负载增加偏差干扰效应增强。研究说明视觉工作记忆负载对听觉偏差干扰的调控是有条件性的,当来自不同感觉通道的无关刺激与目标的加工过程存在重叠时,视觉工作记忆负载的增加使得听觉偏差干扰效应增强而不是减弱。 The present study addressed the controversy around the effect of a working memory load and cognitive distraction, and more specifically the finding in past work that this load sometimes increases distraction, sometimes reduces it. SanMiguel et al. (2010) found that the effect of unexpected novel sounds on performance in a visual working memory task reduced when the task placed significant demands on executive processing by imposing a memory load approaching capacity limits. This load reduced the involuntary orienting of attention toward the sounds and, in turn, their impact (facilitation or distraction) on behavioral performance. In contrast, Lavie (2005) argues that a working memory load increases distraction because load competes for resources with executive control mechanism that attempt to inhibit prepotent responses towards distractors and help limit distraction. We argue that there are two important factors that might influence the deviance distraction: whether distractors are inhibited because they provide no relevant information for the task (e.g., when cueing targets with 50% validity), and whether a load depletes resources otherwise needed for this inhibition. We predicted deviance distraction when attentional resources are directed toward the distractors because they are informative and whena load monopolizes resources otherwise used to inhibit uninformative distractors. In our experiments, participants performed a two-alternative categorization task in which they indicated whether a target stimulus appeared above or below the vertical centre of a computer screen. These targets were preceded by a location cue and, shortly after, a bilateral auditory distractor (standard sound on most trials, deviant sound on the others). Across experiments we manipulated the degree of validity of the location cue (50% or 80%) and whether participants performed the task with a load (1-back task) or without. The location cue was uninformative (50% valid) in Experiments 1 and 2, and informative (80% valid) in Experiments 3 and 4. A load was introduced in Experiments 2 and 4 by use of a 1-back task in which participants responded to the previous trial instead of the current one. The results from Experiment 1 revealed no deviance distraction but longer response times for valid trials, suggesting the inhibition of the distractors. Remarkably, the introduction of a load in Experiment 2 led to observation of deviance distraction, suggesting that the load used resources no longer available for the executive control mechanisms inhibiting responses towards the distractors. In Experiment 3, where location cues were 80% valid, deviance distraction was observed in the absence of a load. Interestingly, it remained so in the presence of a load (Experiment 4). Our results demonstrate that with peripheral visual cues can mediate the extent to which auditory distractors are processed. Distraction seems to appear when attentional resources are available to be directed toward deviant sounds.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2013年第3期263-275,共13页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 中央高校基本科研业务费专项资金资助(10SSXT114) 高等学校博士学科点专项科研基金博导类资助课题(20110043110012)
关键词 外周视线索 偏差干扰 工作记忆负载 视听Oddball范式 periphery visual cue deviant distraction working memory load audio-visual oddball task
  • 相关文献

参考文献2

二级参考文献27

  • 1张明,陈骐.注意定势及时间不确定性对基于空间的返回抑制的影响[J].应用心理学,2002,8(3):15-21. 被引量:8
  • 2Berti, S.(2008). Cognitive control after distraction: Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) dissociate between different processes of attentional allocation. Psychophysiology, 45, 608-620.
  • 3Berti, S., & Schr-ger, E.(2006). Visual distraction: A behavioral and event-related brain potential study in humans. Neuroreport, 17, 151-155.
  • 4Gordon, I. E.(1967). Stimulus probability and simple reaction time. Nature, 215, 895-896.
  • 5N--t-nen, R.(1970). The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods. Acta Psychologica, 34, 399-419.
  • 6Parmentier, F. B. R.(2008). Towards a cognitive model of distraction by auditory novelty: The role of involuntary attention capture and semantic processing. Cognition, 109, 345-362.
  • 7Parmentier, F. B. R., Elsley, J. V., Andrés, P., & Barceló, F. (2011a). Why are auditory novels distracting- Contrasting the roles of novelty, violation of expectation and stimulus change. Cognition, 119(3), 374-380.
  • 8Pomer, M I , & Cohen, Y. Components of visual orienting. In: H Bouma & D G Bohuis eds, Attention and PerformanceX. Hillsdale, NJ:Erlbaum, 1984 : 531- 5562 .
  • 9Terry K M , Valdes, L A ,Neill, W T . Does "inhibition of return" occur in discrimination tasks? Perception & Psychophysics, 1994, (55) : 279 - 286.
  • 10Tanaka, Y , Shimojo, S H. Location vs. Feature: Reaction tim ereveals dissociation between two visual functions. Vision Reseach,1996, (36) :2125 - 2140.

共引文献8

同被引文献16

  • 1Berti, S. ( 2008 ). Cognitive control after distraction: Event - re- lated brain potentials (ERPs)dissociate between different processes of attentional allocation. Psychophysiology, 45,608 -620.
  • 2Berti, S. , & Schroger, E. (2003). Working memory controls in- voluntary attention switching:evidenee from an auditory dis- traction paradigm. European Journal of Neuroscience , 17,1119 -1122.
  • 3Friedman, D. , Cycowicz, Y. M. , & Gaeta, H. (2001). The nov- elty P3 : an event - related brain potential (ERP) sign of the brain' s evaluation of novelty. Neurosci Biobehav, 25,355 - 373.
  • 4Kimura, M. , Katayama, J. I. , & Murohashi, H. ( 2008 ). Underly- ing mechanisms of the P3a task - difficulty effect. Psychophys- iology,45,731 - 741.
  • 5Knight,R. T. (1997). Distributed cortical networks for visual hippocampal region to novelty detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience , 9 , 7 5 - 91.
  • 6Muller - Gass, A. , & Schroger, E. ( 2007 ). Perceptual and cog- nitive task difficulty has differential effects on auditory dis- traction. Brain Research, 1136,169 - 177.
  • 7Munka, L. , & Berti, S. (2006). Examining task - dependencies of different attentional processes as reflected in the P3a and reorienting negativity components of the human event - related brain potential. Neuroscience Letters ,396,177 - 181.
  • 8Naatanen, R. , Paavilainen, P. , Rinne, T. , & Alho, K. ( 2007 ). The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: A review. Clinical Neurophysiology, 118, 2544 - 2590.
  • 9Polo, M. D. , Escera, C. , Yago, E. , Alho, K. , Gual, A. , & Grau, C. (2003). Electrophysiological evidence of abnormal activa- tion of the cerebral network of involuntary attention in alcohol- ism. Clinical Neurophysiology , 114,134 - 146.
  • 10Restuccia, D. , Della Marca, G. , Marra, C. , Rubino, M. , & Vale- riani, M. (2005). Attentional load of the primary task influ- ences the frontal but not the temporal generators of mismatch negativity. Cognitive Brain Research,25,891 - 899.

引证文献4

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部