期刊文献+

对信息源的正向跟随倾向对决策效果的影响 被引量:7

The Positive Following Bias of Information Source and Its Impact on Decision-maker’s Prediction Performance
下载PDF
导出
摘要 采用实验研究的方法,检验了二元选择情形下高准确率信息源(准确率x>50%)和低准确率信息源(准确率为1x)对决策者预测效果的影响。尽管从逻辑上二者从准确率层面具有完全相等的信息价值,然而研究结果显示,决策者更倾向于采用正向跟随而非逆向跟随的方式处理来自信息源的信息。这导致低准确信息源的价值未能得以充分利用。学习和信息源的规范性对该信息处理倾向起到调节作用。 In a statistical sense, individuals might make perfectly correct judgments by using information sources that always offer the wrong predictions, by simply doing the opposite of what the source recommends. Yet despite the statistical feasibility of this option, consumers do not seem to routinely follow information sources that are consistently wrong. Drawing on existing decision making research, especially that related to the status quo effect and positive confirmation bias (PCB), we argue that consumers start with a desire to test the "rightness" instead of the "wrongness" of an information source, which makes them more sensitive to accurate predictions than to errors. Overlooking errors has little influence on information sources with a high forecasting accuracy but could lead to systematically poor predictions about information sources with low forecasting accuracy. However, as consumers gradually disconfirm the default hypothesis with repeated feedback information, they likely assign more weight to the disconfirming information, which lessens bias in their memory and recall of incorrect predictions. If they have access to a high accuracy information source though, learning cannot reduce their bias, because the correct forecasts only strengthen consumers' beliefs about their default hypothesis. Therefore, we also propose that a longer learning period improves the forecast performance of consumers exposed to a low accuracy information source condition but not those exposed to a high accuracy information source. We also argue that individuals are more likely to develop PCB when following a more normative information source, thus informativeness bias might be heightened. Three studies show that decision makers tend to follow information sources with high accuracy rates, even though an information source with low accuracy offers the same level of informativeness for decision making. This positive following bias can lead people to underestimate the value of information from low accuracy information sources. Learning can reduce this positive following bias phenomenon. Furthermore, the positive following bias is more likely to occur for a more normative information source. An alternative explanation for the positive following bias explanation might be variance in information could influence consumers' assessments of its value, such that consumers infer that a low accuracy rate implies greater variance or less reliability. In turn, they may decide that incorrect predictions are abnormal and not informative. Therefore, as a comprehensive test of the informativeness bias explanation, we also rule out the influence of the information variance factor by introducing a zero variance s, that is, when a person consistently makes wrong predictions. Our findings also contribute to judgment and choice research. First, to the best of our knowledge, no other research has addressed biased acquisitions and the use of high error rate information, despite recognition of the value of this type of information in prior theoretical analyses. We test for the existence of an informativeness bias in binary choices situations and examine potential moderators of this effect. Second, we extend PCB research by examining its role as an antecedent of an informativeness bias. Third, we find that learning, previous experience, and information source normativeness have not only direct effects for reducing informativeness bias but also help clarify research on the regulation of PCB.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2013年第8期887-898,共12页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 国家自然科学基金(71172011 71272044) 教育部人文社科基金(11YJC630183)资助
关键词 信息处理偏见 预测准确率 信息源 positive following bias prediction accuracy rate information source
  • 相关文献

参考文献48

  • 1Baird, R. R. (2003). Experts sometimes show more false recall than novices: A cost of knowing too much. Learning and Individual Differences, 3, 349-355.
  • 2Baron, J. (1991). Beliefs about thinking. In J. F. Voss, D. N. Perkins, & J. W. Segal (Eds.), Informal Reasoning and Education (pp. 169-186). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • 3Budescu, D. V., & Maciejovsky, B. (2005). The effect of payoff feedback and information pooling on reasoning errors: Evidence from experimental markets. Management Science, 51, 1829-1843.
  • 4Castel, A. D., McCabe, D. P., Roediger Iii, H. L., & Heitman, J. L. (2007). The dark side of expertise: Domain-specific memory errors. Psychological Science, 18, 3-5.
  • 5Chaffee, S. H., & Miyo, Y. (1983). Selective exposure and the reinforcement hypothesis: An intergenerational panel study of the 1980 presidential campaign. Communication Research, 10, 3-36.
  • 6Chandrashekaran, M., McNeilly, K., Russ, F. A., & Marinova, D. (2000). From uncertain intentions to actual behavior: A threshold model of whether and when salespeople quit. Journal of Marketing Research, 37, 463-479.
  • 7Chandrashekaran, M., Rotte, K., Tax, S. S., & Grewal, R. (2007). Satisfaction strength and customer loyalty. Journal of Marketing Research, 44, 153-163.
  • 8Christandl, F., Fetchenhauer, D., & Hoelzl, E. (2011). Price perception and confirmation bias in the context of a VAT increase. Journal of Economic Psychology, 2, 131-141.
  • 9DellaVigna, S. (2009). Psychology and economics: Evidence from the field. Journal of Economic Literature, 47, 315-372.
  • 10Dana, J., Weber, R. A., & Kuang, J. X. (2007). Exploiting moral wiggle room: Experiments demonstrating an illusory preference for fairness. Economic Theory, 33, 67-80.

同被引文献88

引证文献7

二级引证文献277

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部