期刊文献+

类别型状态不确定独立否定句的加工机制:来自眼动实验的证据 被引量:5

The Processing of Isolated Negative Sentences with Uncertain Categorical States: Evidence from Eye-movement Experiments
下载PDF
导出
摘要 探讨类别型状态不确定独立否定句(如"裙子不是蓝色的")的动态表征过程。实验1和实验2分别仅给被试提供"与事件备择选项一致的图片"(红色裙子)和"含有否定标签的与事件否定状态一致的图片"(含有否定标签的蓝色裙子);实验3同时提供以上两种选项。结果发现:3个实验中被试在早期均对"与事件否定状态一致的图片"注视概率更高,在晚期阶段,实验1仅对"与事件备择选项一致的图片"注视概率更高,实验2和实验3均仅对"含有否定标签的与事件否定状态一致的图片"注视概率更高,并且选择为事件的最终状态;实验4的评定结果表明,当被试有充裕时间完成任务时,37名评定者中有30名选择"与事件备择选项一致的图片"为事件的最终状态。4个实验的结果为锚激活与限制满足模型和抑制假设提供了进一步证据。 Affirmation and negation is two main semantic and grammatical categories in any language. In order to explain the processing mechanism of negative sentence, some researchers have proposed schema-plus-tag model and two-step simulation hypotheses. However, these hypotheses cannot explain the processing mechanism of isolated negative sentences with uncertain states, such as the skirt is not blue. So anchor-based activation and satisfaction-constrained model was proposed to solve this problem. In the present study, we used eye-tracking to give further support to this new model. In the three experiments, a visual-world paradigm was adopted to explore the processing mechanism of negative sentences with uncertain states under three different contexts. In Experiment 1, participants were only presented with the alternative choice of the depicted event (e.g., red skirt), short for ‘A'; In Experiment 2 only presented ‘NOT X' (e.g., blue skirt attaching negative marker); In Experiment 3 presented both ‘A' and ‘NOT X' simultaneously. In these three eye-tracking experiments, participants were first presented with the sentences by voice, and then at 1200 ms, four pictures were presented simultaneously at the beginning of the state word (e.g., blue). And participants' task was to choose which picture matched the sentence. The materials of Experiment 4 were adopted from Experiment 3, however, presented by offiine. So, participants have plenty of time to choose the matching pictures. The results demonstrated that, at the early stage of processing, participants had higher fixation probabilities to pictures depicting the negated state of affairs, such as 'blue skirt' in all the three experiments. Then at the later stage, participants showed higher fixation probabilities to the pictures depicting‘A' in Experiment 1; however, to ‘NOT X' in Experiment 2 and 3. In addition, participants showed lower fixation probabilities to the pictures depicting the negated states than the random level. In Experiment 4, 30 out of 37 participants chose the pictures depicting ‘A' rather than ‘NOT X' being the matching pictures. The results from all four experiments showed that, when processing the negative sentences with uncertain states, participants would first simulate an anchor (representation of the negated state of affairs), then searched for the actual state of affairs based on the given context. If participants had enough time to get the actual state of affairs based on linguistic features and the given context, then the searching stopped and accepted the actual state as the final state of affairs. However, if participants cannot get the actual state of affairs based on linguistic features and the context or had not enough time to finish the whole process, then the searching also stopped and accepted the representation of the negated state of affairs attaching negative marker being the processing result. At the later stage, the representation of negated state of affairs would be suppressed for useless. The results support anchor-based activation and satisfaction constrained model as well as the suppression hypothesis.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2014年第10期1426-1441,共16页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 2010教育部新世纪优秀人才支持计划项目(项目号:NCET-10-0089)
关键词 类别型状态不确定独立否定句 视觉-情境范式 锚激活与限制满足模型 negation isolated negative sentences with indefinite states anchor-based activation and satisfaction-constrained model visual-world paradigm
  • 相关文献

参考文献29

  • 1Salverda, A., Dahan, D., & McQueen, J. M. (2003). The role of prosodic boundaries in the resolution of lexical embedding in speech comprehension. Cognition, 90, 51-89.
  • 2Giora, R., Heruti, V., Metuki, N., & Fein, O. (2009). "When we say no we mean no": Interpreting negation in vision and language. Journal of Pragmatics, 41, 2222-2239.
  • 3Fiedler, K., Walther, E., Armbruster, T., Fay, D., & Naumarm, U. (1996). Do you really know what you have seen? Intrusion errors and presuppositions effects on constructive memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 484-511.
  • 4Giora, R., & Fein, O. (2007). Negation in context: A functional approach to suppression. Discourse Processes, 43(2): 153-172.
  • 5Morrow, D. G, Bower, Cx H., & Greenspan, S. L. (1990). Situation-based inferences during narrative comprehension. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 25, 123-135.
  • 6Hasson, l., & Glucksberg S. (2"006i. Does understanding negation entail affirmation? Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 1015-1032.
  • 7高志华,鲁忠义,马红霞.汉语简单否定陈述句理解的心理模拟过程[J].心理学报,2011,43(12):1380-1387. 被引量:11
  • 8Zanuttini, R. (1997).Negation and clausal structure." A comparative study of Romance languages. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • 9邱丽景,王穗苹,关心.口语理解的视觉—情境范式研究[J].华南师范大学学报(社会科学版),2009(1):130-136. 被引量:9
  • 10He, X. Y., Lin, X. M., Li, H. J., Chen, G. Y., & Yang, H. (2013).New Progresses of Research on Negative Processing: From the Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective. Journal of South China Normal University (Social Science Edition): (1): 33-41.

二级参考文献84

  • 1张琦.英汉否定之比较[J].内蒙古民族大学学报(社会科学版),1998,25(1):60-61. 被引量:1
  • 2张有军.语言认知的哲学观[J].辽宁大学学报(哲学社会科学版),2004,32(5):18-20. 被引量:4
  • 3王寅.认知语言学与两代认知科学[J].外语学刊,2002(1):9-14. 被引量:49
  • 4TANENHAUS M K, SPIVEY - KNOWLTON, EBERHARD K M, SEDIBY J C. Integration of Visual and Linguistic Information in Spoken Language Comprehension. Science, 1995(5217), 268:1632 - 1634.
  • 5TANENHAUS M K, MAGNUSON J S, DAHAN D, CHAMBERS C. Eye Movements and Lexical Access in Spoken - Language Comprehension: Evaluating a Linking Hypothesis between Fixations and Linguistic Proceesing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2000, 29(6) : 557 -580.
  • 6CLARK H H, CARLSON T B. Hearers and Speech Acts. Language, 1982, 58(2) : 332-373.
  • 7MARSLEN- WILSON W. Functional Parallelism in Spoken Word Recognition. Cognition, 1987, 25 ( 1 - 2) : 71 - 102.
  • 8MCCLELLAND J L, ELMAN J L. The Trace Model of Speech Perception. Cognitive Psychology, 1986, 18 (1): 1 -86.
  • 9NORRIS D. Shortlist: A Connectionist Model of Continuous Speech Recognition. Cognition, 1994, 52 (3) : 189 -234.
  • 10ALLOPENNA P D, MAGNUSON J S, TANENHAUS M K. Tracking the Time Course of Spoken Word Recognition Using Eye Movements: Evidence for Continuous Mapping Models. Journal of Memory and Lan guage, 1998, 38(4) : 419 -439.

共引文献28

同被引文献29

  • 1李莹,王瑞明,莫雷.否定句理解中知觉仿真的动态过程[J].心理科学,2007,30(4):791-795. 被引量:10
  • 2王寅.认知语言学写两代认知科学[J]外语学刊,2002(01).
  • 3Wason,P.C.Responses to affirmative and negative binary statements[J].British Journal of Psychology,1961(52).
  • 4Schmalhofer,F.&Perfetti,C.A.Higher language processes in the brain[J].Journal of Erlbaum,2007(1).
  • 5Barsalou,L.W.Perceptual Symbol System[J].Behavioral and Brain Sciences,1999(22).
  • 6Kaup,B.,Ludtke,J.&Zwaan,R.A.Processing negated sentences with contradictory predicates:Is a door that is not open mentally closed?[J].Journal of Pragmatics,2006(38).
  • 7Macdonald,M.C.&Just,M.A.Changes in activation levels with negation[J].Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning,Memory,and Cognition,1989(15).
  • 8Kaup,B.&Zwaan,R.A.Effects of negation and situational presence on the accessibility of text information[J].Journal of Experimental Psychology:Learning,Memory and Cognition,2003(3).
  • 9Hasson,U.&Glucksberg,S.Does understanding negation entail affirmation?——An examination of negated metaphors[J].Journal of Pragmatics,2006(38).
  • 10Orenes,I.,Beltrán,D.&Santamaría,C.How negation is understood:Evidence from the visual world paradigm[J].Journal of Memory and Language,2014(74).

引证文献5

二级引证文献9

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部