期刊文献+

小学生领导力萌芽与发展的实证研究 被引量:2

Emergence and Development of Primary School Children's Leadership in Group Discussions
下载PDF
导出
摘要 本研究以四年级学生为研究对象,通过话语分析,探究了八个无领导小组八次讨论过程中领导力的萌芽状况及其影响因素。结果发现,小学生可自发出现领导力行为,但只有两个小组会涌现出明显的领导者,并表现为独立领导和协同领导两种不同模式。对学生和教师行为的综合分析显示,学生领导力的形成与教师示范、反馈及同伴互动存在密切关系,这对学生领导力的培养具有重要的启示意义。 Emergent leadership was examined in 8 groups implementing Collaborative Reasoning (CR) discussions in 4 fourth-grade classrooms from a primary school in China's Mainland. Children's leadership moves were coded from 63 free-flowing, open-format discussions transcripts, and each of the 8 groups have 8 discussions separately (one discussion transcript was missing). The transcripts encompassed 22 hours and 38 minutes with 6178 turns for speaking, which were evaluated to belong to one of the 13 leadership functions: Turn Management, Voting Organization, Reminding Oround Rules, Initiation Task, Spontaneous Responses and Encouraging/Acknowledgement, Reminding, Asking for Clarification, Topic Control, Asking for Reasons, Thinking aloud, Challenging, Summing up. Comparison of the number and kind of leadership moves made by the children showed that there were emergent leadership moves in each of the 8 collaborative reasoning discussion groups, but only two groups leaders emerged in 2 out of the 8 groups, while one primary leader emerged in one group and leadership responsibilities were shared among three children in another group. Compared with the previous research about the emergent leadership among the American primary students in CR discussion, the leadership patterns of Chinese children were similar to that of American children, but the frequency of leadership moves of the Chinese fourth graders were much lower than that of their peers from America. To better understand how the different leaders grew in their groups, those group leaders of the two groups and their interaction with the teacher and other students were investigated in detail. The results showed that the frequency and kind of leadership moves of the primary leader followed the patterns of her teacher, which increased with the progression of the discussions at the beginning and stabled at a low level at the end, suggesting that the emerging leaders were learning how to lead from her teacher. Encouragement and acknowledgement from the teacher played a big role in the growth of student leaders. Analyses of the three leaders sharing the leadership responsibilities indicated that these students who were descripted by their teacher as talkative and who were regarded as having good ideas, exhibited more leadership than other children. This study confirmed our hypothesis that the Collaborative Reasoning discussions provided an environment conducive to the emergence of child leaders and the acquisition and refinement of leadership schemas. Features of Collaborative Reasoning discussions--specifically, the teacher providing social and intellectual space, open participation, and mastery orientation--enable children to have more control over group dynamics. As time proceeds, child leaders gradually take over aspects of leadership conventionally performed by teachers and organize the discussion in an effective way. The findings in the present study had important implications for classroom implementations to develop children's leadership in China.
出处 《心理科学》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2015年第3期612-617,共6页 Journal of Psychological Science
基金 教育部重大攻关项目(11JZD041) 中央高校基本科研业务专项基金的资助
关键词 协作推理讨论 小学生 领导力 质性分析 collaborative reasoning discussion, students, leadership, qualitative analyze
  • 相关文献

参考文献12

  • 1任真,王登峰.中国领导心理与行为实证研究二十年进展[J].心理学探新,2008,28(1):67-71. 被引量:9
  • 2田效勋,车宏生.行为面试与无领导小组讨论的预测效度研究[J].心理科学,2009,32(1):187-189. 被引量:5
  • 3Almasi, J. F., O' Flahavan, J. F., & Arya, P. (2001) A comparative analysis of student and teacher development in more and less proficient discussions of literature. Reacb'ng Research Quarterly, 36, 96-120.
  • 4Anderson, R. C., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., McNurlen, B., Archodidou, A., Kim, S., et al. (2001). The snowball phenomenon: Spad of ways of talking and ways of thinking across groups of children. Cognition and Instruction, 19, 1-46.
  • 5Anderson, R. C., & Pearson, P. D. (1984). A schema-theoretic view of basic processes in reading comprehension. In P. D. Pearson, R. Barr, M. L. Kamil, & P. Mosemhal (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (pp. 255-291). New York: Longman.
  • 6Carte, T. A., Chidambaram, L., & Becker, A. (2006). Emergent leadership in self- managed virtual teams. Group Decision and Negoagon, 15(4), 323-343.
  • 7French, D. C., & Stright, A. L (1991). Emergent leadership in children' s small groups. Small Group Research, 22, 187-199.
  • 8Kickul, J., & Neuman, G. (2000). Emergent leadership behaviors: The function of personality and cognitive ability in determining teamwork performance and KSAs. Journal oBuslness and Psychology, 15(1), 27-51.
  • 9Li, Y., Anderson, R. C., Nguyen-Jahiel, K., Dong, ., Arehodidou, A., et at. (2007). Emergent leadership in children' s discussion groups .Cognition and Instruction, 25, 75-111.
  • 10Miller, B. (2010). Child leaders in collaborative groups. In C. Hmelo-Silver, A. O'Dormell, C. Chan, & C. Chinn(Eds.), International handbook ocollaborative learning (pp. 268-278). London: Taylor & Francis.

二级参考文献44

共引文献12

同被引文献11

引证文献2

二级引证文献1

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部