期刊文献+

权力感和任务难度对个体建议采纳的影响 被引量:6

The Effect of Power Level and Task Difficulty on Advice Taking
下载PDF
导出
摘要 从人际关系的视角,通过两个实验探讨权力感和任务难度对个体建议采纳的影响。实验1启动被试的合理权力感,实验2启动被试的不合理权力感。研究发现:(1)当权力合理时,高权力感水平决策者的建议采纳程度显著低于低权力感水平的决策者;决策者在困难任务中的建议采纳程度显著高于简单任务中。(2)当权力不合理时,在简单任务中,高权力感水平决策者的建议采纳程度显著低于低权力感水平的决策者;而在困难任务中,两者的建议采纳程度没有显著性差异。 In the domain of advice taking, most research focuses on how others' advice influences the advisee's decision and the extent to which the advisee accepts the advice. Advice taking is an interpersonal process in which the social status of adviser and judge plays an important role. In such a setting, power may convey important information about the relationship between the adviser and the advisee. So the perceived power of on the part of the advisee may affect the degree of advice taking. Because individuals who experience high level of power have more confidence, and tend to keep distance from others according to the social distance theory of power, individuals with high level of subjective sense of power tend to discount advice, which is stated by previous research. But the legitimacy of power may decrease the confidence and reduce the social distance of the advisee. That is, the influence of power on behaviors will change if individuals think the power is illegitimate, so the effect of illegal power on advice taking would be reversed. What's more, task difficulty may also moderate the effect of power on advice taking. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to investigate the influence of different power and task difficulty on advice taking. Two experiments were designed within the JAS paradigm. The decision task was estimating the number of coins in a glass. The legitimate power in experiment 1 and the illegitimate power in experiment 2 were primed by using word search tasks. There were 122 college students taking part in experiment 1 and 97 students taking part in experiment 2. The dependent variable was the level of advice-taking which was calculated by the formula of WOA (weight of advice). The independent variables were power level (high vs. low) and task difficulty (easy vs. difficult). Dependent variable was the extent of advice taking, and estimation task was used in both experiments. The results were as follow: (1) When advisees thought their power were legitimate, their level of power significantly influenced the level of advice taking. Individuals experiencing high level of power were less receptive to advice than individuals experiencing low level of power. Task difficulty had significant effects on advice taking. People weighed advice more significantly when the task was difficult than when the task was easy. (2) When the advisees thought their power was illegitimate, individuals experiencing high level of power weighted advice less than individuals experiencing low level of power when the task was easy, but there was no difference between them when the task was difficult. This research suggested that the power status unconsciously led to reject others' advice, although everyone knew that the value of listening to both viewpoints to get better informed. So we should remind ourselves constantly not treat others by condescendingly. A modest attitude to others' advice is conducive to rational decision making.
出处 《心理科学》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2016年第5期1184-1189,共6页 Journal of Psychological Science
基金 山东省自然科学基金项目(ZR2015CM026)经费的资助
关键词 权力感 合理性 任务难度 建议采纳 experienced power, legitimacy, task difficulty, advice taking
  • 相关文献

参考文献29

  • 1段锦云,黄彩云.个人权力感对进谏行为的影响机制:权力认知的视角[J].心理学报,2013,45(2):217-230. 被引量:73
  • 2张艳梅,杜秀芳,王修欣.焦虑、建议者善意程度对个体建议采纳的影响[J].心理科学,2015,38(5):1155-1161. 被引量:12
  • 3Atwater, L. E., Wang, M., Smith, J. W., & Fleenor, J. W. (2009). Are cultural characteristics associated with the relationship between self and others' rating of leadership? Journal o~Applied Psychology, 94(4), 876-886.
  • 4Brifiol, P., Petty, R. E., Valle, C., Rucker, D. D., & Beccera, A. (2007). The eff~ts of message recipients' power ~fore and after persuasion: A self-validation analysis. Journal of Personality and Social P~yebolob, y, 93(6), 1040-1053.
  • 5DeCelles, K. A., DeRue, D. S., Margolis, J. D., & Ceranic, T. L. (2012). D~s power corrupt or enable? When and why power facilitates self-interested behavior. Journal o~ Applied Psycbology, 97(3), 681-689.
  • 6Fast, N. J., Sivanathan, N., Mayer, N. D., & Galinsky, A. D. (2012). Power and overeonfident decision-making. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Precesses, 117(2), 249-260.
  • 7Fiske, S. T. (2010). Interpersonal stratification: Status, power, and subordination. In Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), H~mdbook ofs~ial psycbolosy ( pp. 941-982). New York: Wiley.
  • 8Fragale, A. R. (2006). The power of powerless speech: The effects of speech style and task interdependence on status conferral. Organizadon~d Behavior and Human D~islon Processes, 101 (2), 243-261.
  • 9Georgesen, J. C., & Harris, M. J. (1998). Why's my boss always holding me down? A meta-analysis of power effects on performance evaluations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 2(3), 184-195.
  • 10Gino, F., Brooks, A. W. & ~hweitzer, M. E. (2012). Anxiety, advice, and the ability to discern: Feeling anxious motivates individuals to seek and use advice. Journal of Personality and Social P~yehology, 102(3), 497-512.

二级参考文献101

  • 1马庆钰.中国政治文化论纲[J].理论学刊,2002(6):29-32. 被引量:12
  • 2梁建,王重鸣.中国背景下的人际关系及其对组织绩效的影响[J].心理学动态,2001,9(2):173-178. 被引量:42
  • 3温忠麟,张雷,侯杰泰.有中介的调节变量和有调节的中介变量[J].心理学报,2006,38(3):448-452. 被引量:733
  • 4段锦云,王重鸣,钟建安.大五和组织公平感对进谏行为的影响研究[J].心理科学,2007,30(1):19-22. 被引量:84
  • 5Alexander, J. K., Hillier, A., Smith, R. M., Tivarus, M. E., & Beversdorf, D. Q. (2007). Beta-adrenergic modulation of cognitive flexibility during stress. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(3), 468-478.
  • 6Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
  • 7Anderson, C., & Berdahl, J. L. (2002). The experience of power: Examining the effects of power on approach and inhibition tendencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1362-1377.
  • 8Anderson, C., & Galinsky, A. D. (2006). Power, optimism, and risk-taking. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36(4), 511-536.
  • 9Anderson, C., John, O. P., & Keltner, D. (2012). The personal sense of power. Journal of Personality, 80(2), 313-344.
  • 10Antonakis, J., & Atwater, L. (2002). Leader distance: A review and a proposed theory. Leadership Quarterly, 13(6), 673-704.

共引文献83

同被引文献77

引证文献6

二级引证文献24

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部