期刊文献+

4~6岁幼儿对口语和文字信息的信任及运用 被引量:2

Trust and subsequent use of oral and print information for 4~6 year-old children
下载PDF
导出
摘要 研究以混合物分类为实验任务,探讨在面对口语和文字两种信息时,4~6岁中国幼儿的信任特点,在信任任务之后的即时和延时两个时间点上考察幼儿对所信任信息的运用,以及分析阅读水平对幼儿信息信任和运用的影响。研究结果表明:(1)4~6岁中国幼儿对不同类型信息都表现出高度信任,相较于口语信息,阅读水平高的幼儿表现出对文字信息的信任偏好;(2)幼儿在后续任务中较少运用所信任的信息,随着时间的推移,幼儿运用的口语信息持续减少,但文字信息的运用呈现先降后升的特点;(3)阅读水平在不同时间点上影响幼儿对所信任信息的运用,阅读水平低的幼儿在短时间内更多地运用口语信息,而阅读水平高的幼儿在更长时间里显著更多地使用文字信息。未来研究应进一步探究文字意识对幼儿文字信息信任和后续运用的作用。 Children have many opportunities to learn from others through oral and printed sources. A few recent studies investigating children's trust in printed versus oral information suggested that as soon as children acquired the basic reading ability, they placed more trust in printed over oral testimony when learning names for unfamiliar objects. Previous studies have shown that a proportion of the information that early readers gained from printed sources might be fragile and lost once the printed label is no longer present, but those studies have not examined young children's subsequent use of the accepted oral or printed information. The current experiments examine whether Chinese young children from 4 to 6 years old trust the printed information more than oral information, to what extent the young children extend their new knowledge to subsequent circumstances, and the effects of reading ability on initial trust and subsequent use. In this study, 125 Chinese young children from 4 to 6 years of age were tested. Each child completed four tasks: the reading ability task, the hybrid sorting task, the short-term recognition task of hybrid pictures (3 minutes after the sorting task), and the long-term recognition task (2 weeks later). Children's reading ability was evaluated by a single-word recognition test involving words from the hybrid sorting task. For the hybrid sorting task, children were presented with pictures of hybrid creatures that looked more like one of two species, and had the opportunity to accept or reject an oral or printed label that referred to the perceptually non-dominant species. For the recognition tasks, children were asked to name the same set of hybrid creatures or sort them to the related locations. We analyzed the experimental data using hierarchical linear modeling (HLM) (score level and individual level) to investigate the contribution of information type, reading ability and age to children's initial trust of information, children's recognition of hybrid pictures in the short term and in the long term. We found that Chinese young children trusted both oral and printed labels in the original sorting tasks: There's no significant main effects of information type, age, or reading ability on children's trust in oral or printed information, but the interaction between reading ability and information type predicts children's trust. Using simple slope analysis we found that all children trusted the printed information, while higher-level readers were more likely to reject the oral information than lower-level readers. Despite of prior great trust of information in the original sorting tasks, young children seldom applied the information to the subsequent short-term or long-term tasks. However, they tended to apply more printed information than oral information in the long run. There were interaction effects of reading ability and information type on children's subsequent use of information both in the short-term and in the long- term. Specifically, lower level readers were more likely to use the oral information than the higher level readers in the short term, while higher-level reader were more likely to use the printed information than the lower-level readers in the long run. The role of print awareness in the relationship between reading ability and children's trust and subsequent use of information was discussed.
出处 《心理学报》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2017年第8期1053-1062,共10页 Acta Psychologica Sinica
基金 浙江省自然科学基金青年项目(LQ13C090003) 浙江省哲学社会科学基金项目(14NDJC221YB) 浙江省教育厅一般科研项目(Y201534265)资助
关键词 信任 口语信息 文字信息 阅读水平 trust spoken information printed information reading ability
  • 相关文献

参考文献3

二级参考文献162

  • 1彭聃龄,刘颖,陈鹰.汉字识别的计算机模拟[J].应用心理学,1996,2(1):9-16. 被引量:7
  • 2刘鸣.汉字认知与形象思维[J].华南师范大学学报(社会科学版),1996(4):46-51. 被引量:10
  • 3刘翔平,刘文理,张立娜,徐先金,张微,张秀秀,张婧乔.儿童识字能力与汉字规则意识关系研究[J].中国特殊教育,2006(1):56-61. 被引量:14
  • 4李虹,彭虹,舒华.汉语儿童正字法意识的萌芽与发展[J].心理发展与教育,2006,22(1):35-38. 被引量:58
  • 5郝美玲.留学生汉字正字法意识的萌芽与发展[J].世界汉语教学,2007,21(1):29-39. 被引量:52
  • 6舒华.汉语儿童词汇和阅读获得的研究[A].载彭聃龄编.《汉语认知研究》[C].山东教育出版社,1997..
  • 7Li Y, Kang J S. Analysis of phonetics of semantic-phonetic compound characters in modern Chinese. In: Chen Y ed. Information analysis of usage of characters in modern Chinese. Shanghai Education press, 1993. 29-36 (李燕, 康加深. 现代汉语形声字声符研究. 见: 陈源主编. 现代汉语用字信息分析. 上海教育出版社, 1993. 29-36)
  • 8Chan, L., & Nunes, T. (1998). Children's understanding of the formal and functional characteristics of written Chinese. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 115-132.
  • 9Chen, Y., Allport, D., & Marshall, J. (1996). What are the functional orthographic units in Chinese word recognition: The stroke or the stroke pattern? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 49(4), 1024-1043.
  • 10Clay,-M. M. (1993). An observation survey of early literacy achievement. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

共引文献40

同被引文献5

引证文献2

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部