期刊文献+

控制感剥夺激发状态毅力:趋近动机的中介作用 被引量:5

Control Deprivation Stimulates State Grit: The Mediating Effects of Approach Motivation
下载PDF
导出
摘要 采用概念判断任务,通过两个递进式实验探索控制感剥夺对状态毅力的影响并考察趋近动机在其中的作用。实验1主观报告结果显示:短时条件下,控制感剥夺组的状态毅力显著高于控制感维持组,尤其表现在努力持续性维度;长时条件下,两组差异不显著。实验2在此基础上加入行为指标结果显示:短时控制感剥夺通过高趋近动机对状态毅力产生影响。结果表明毅力是可塑的,突出表现在努力持续性上,该效应出现在个体经历短时控制感剥夺后。 The control deprivation has two kinds of effects on individuals: learned helplessness or more courage in dilemma. A literature proposed that brief experiences of control deprivation made people reassert control with more efforts, and prolonged experiences of control deprivation made people give up and cease seeking control. Thus the time length of the control deprivation matters. Prolonged experiences of control deprivation costs much energy which leads to negative consequences while brief experiences of control deprivation bring people with constructive power -- state grit. Approach motivation facilitates goal-realization and may be the mechanism underlying the influence of control deprivation.This study conducted 2 experiments with a Concept-Identification task which examined effects of different types of control deprivation on state grit. Experiment 1 manipulated the length of time (prolonged vs. brief) and the sense of control (maintenance vs. deprivation), 90 undergraduates were randomly allocated into four groups. The state grit was measured by self-report scales (Grit-S). The results of experiment 1 revealed significant interaction effect on state grit between time-length and control types, especially on perseverance of effort factor (p 〈 .05), but not on consistency of interests factor. The simple effect test showed that the effect on perseverance of effort of the short-time group was significantly higher than that of the long-time group (p 〈 .05) under the control deprivation condition. No significant difference between the two groups under the control maintenance condition was showed. The results of experiment 1 indicated that brief experiences of control deprivation created perseverance of effort, which built foundation for experiment 2 to select behavioral measures for perseverance of effort. Experiment 2 conducted a between-group experiment design and manipulated the types of control (brief experiences of control deprivation vs. prolonged experiences of control deprivation) which deeply explored the function of high and low approach motivation, and 67 undergraduates were primed by the two categories. Besides, the experiment 2 had a grip task (the performance of grip was calculated by the length of time: persistence time minus base-lined time). Correlation analysis results showed that the types of control were positively correlated with high approach motivation, perseverance of effort factor and grip grades, and high approach motivation was positively correlated with perseverance of effort factor and grip grades. The results showed that the high approach motivation acted as the mediator between brief experiences of control deprivation and state grit. The findings implied that brief experiences of control deprivation created a state of grit through the high approach motivation, and especially on perseverance of effort. In general, the present study provided supportive evidence of the idea that grit exhibited state-like tendencies and the evidence of making people "grittier" through training, which led researchers to focus on the power of the individuals' potential.
作者 员东婷 王英春 Yun Dongting;Wang Yingchun(School of Psychology,Beijing Sport University,Beijing,100084)
出处 《心理科学》 CSSCI CSCD 北大核心 2018年第5期1192-1199,共8页 Journal of Psychological Science
基金 中央高校基本科研业务费专项基金项目(2016ZX018 2018PT018)的资助
关键词 控制感剥夺 状态毅力 趋近动机 control deprivation state grit approach motivation
  • 相关文献

参考文献6

二级参考文献109

  • 1蒙茜,郑涌.拖延研究述评[J].西南师范大学学报(人文社会科学版),2006,32(4):9-12. 被引量:83
  • 2Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2007). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of Psychophysiology (3rd ed., pp. 581-607). New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • 3Briggs, K. E., & Martin, F. H. (2009). Affective picture processing and motivational relevance: Arousal and valence effects on ERPs in an oddball task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 72, 299-306.
  • 4Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 183-204.
  • 5Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions Revealed. New York: Times Books.
  • 6Franken, I. H. A., Van Strien, J. W., Bocanegra, B. R., & Huijding, J. (2011). The P3 event-related potential as an index of motivational relevance: A conditioning experiment. Journal of Psychophysiology, 25, 32-39.
  • 7Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: The broaden and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56, 218-226.
  • 8Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive emotions broaden the scope of attention and thought-action repertoires. Cognition and Emotion, 19, 313-332.
  • 9Gable, E A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008a). Approach-motivated positive affect reduces breadth of attention. Psychological Science, 19, 476-482.
  • 10Gable, E A., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2008b). Relative left frontal activation to appetitive stimuli: Considering the role of individual differences. Psychophysiology, 45, 275-278.

共引文献144

同被引文献38

引证文献5

二级引证文献10

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部