期刊文献+

高动机情绪对面孔识别整体加工影响的事件相关电位研究

Face holistic processing is modulated by high-motivation intensity: an event-related potential study
原文传递
导出
摘要 目的探讨高动机情绪背景对面孔整体加工的影响及其神经机制。方法26名被试在三种情绪图片(美食、中性、恐惧)启动条件下执行组合面孔任务。结果(1)行为学研究显示,情绪类型与面孔组合形式交互作用显著[F(2,50)=5.37,P=0.008,η^2=0.18]。在中性情绪条件下出现组合面孔效应:被试对错开面孔的辨别准确率[(90.21±11.20)%]显著高于对齐面孔[(84.83±11.20)%](t(25)=3.95,P=0.001)。在高动机情绪条件下组合面孔效应消失:即在高动机消极情绪条件下被试对错开面孔[(87.88±10.28)%]和对齐面孔[(85.12±8.81)%]的辨别准确率差异无统计学意义(t(25)=1.93,P=0.065);在高动机积极情绪条件下被试对错开面孔[(87.92±11.00)%]和对齐面孔[(88.33±8.89)%]的辨别准确率差异无统计学意义(t(25)=0.263,P=0.795)。(2)ERP结果显示:P3成分上情绪类型和面孔组合形式交互作用显著(F(2,50)=3.23,P=0.048,η^2=0.12)。中性情绪条件下,对齐面孔[(3.46±1.26)μV]产生的P3波幅显著大于错开面孔[(2.82±1.43)μV](F(1,25)=5.28,P=0.03,η^2=0.17);高动机消极条件下,对齐面孔[(3.21±1.16)μV]和错开面孔[(3.07±1.22)μV]产生的P3波幅差异无统计学意义(F(1,25)=0.53,P=0.476);高动机积极情绪条件下,对齐面孔[(3.33±1.20)μV]和错开面孔[(3.42±1.40)μV]产生的P3波幅差异也无统计学意义(F(1,25)=0.23,P=0.638)。结论面孔加工与情绪加工之间具有高度关联性,揭示了情绪的动机性对面孔加工有更强的影响。无论情绪效价如何,高动机情绪都能引起局部注意偏向并降低面孔整体加工,且主要表现在晚期认知加工阶段。 Objective To investigate the influence of high-motivated affect background on the holistic face processing and its neural mechanism. Methods Totally 26 subjects were instructed to perform composite-face process under the priming condition of three emotional pictures(food, neutral and fear). Results (1)Behavioral studies showed a significant interaction between emotion type and face combination(F(2, 50)=5.37, P=0.008). Composite-face effect was observed in neutral emotional condition due to the fact that the recognition accuracy of the misaligned face((90.21±11.20)%) was significantly higher than that of the aligned face((84.83±11.20)%, t(25)=3.95, P=0.001). However, the composite-face effect disappeared under the condition of high-motivated emotion. That is, there was no significant difference in the recognition accuracy of the misaligned faces ((87.88±10.28)%) and aligned faces((85.12±8.81)%, t(25)=1.93, P=0.065) under the condition of high-motivated negative emotion. Under the condition of high-motivated positive emotion, there was also no significant difference in the recognition accuracy between the misaligned faces((87.92±11.00)%) and aligned faces((88.33±8.89)%, t(25)=0.263, P=0.795).(2)According to the results of ERP, P3 components showed significant interaction between emotion type and face combination form(F(2, 50)=3.23, P=0.048). Under the neutral emotional condition, the P3 amplitude of aligned faces((3.46±1.26)μV) was significantly higher than that of misaligned faces((2.82±1.43)μV, F(1, 25)=5.28, P=0.03). In the case of high-motivated negative condition, there was no significant difference in P3 amplitude between aligned faces((3.21±1.16)μV) and misaligned faces((3.07±1.22)μV, F(1, 25)=0.53, P=0.476). The P3 amplitude of aligned faces((3.33±1.20)μV) and misaligned faces((3.42±1.40)μV) were also not significantly different under the condition of high-motivated positive affect(F(1, 25)=0.23, P=0.638). Conclusion The study shows a high correlation between face processing and emotional processing.Additionally, the emotional motivation has a stronger influence on face processing. Regardless of the emotional valence, high-motivated intensity affect can cause local attention bias and reduce the holistic processing level of the face, which is mainly manifested in the later cognitive processing stage.
作者 王安琪 朱海东 朱洵 乔亲才 陈谜 毛泽海 Wang Anqi;Zhu Haidong;Zhu Xun;Qiao Qincai;Chen Mi;Mao Zehai(Department of Psychology,Teachers College,Shihezi University,Shihezi 832000,China;Department of Psychological Application Research Center,The Corps Education College of Shihezi University,Shihezi 832000,China)
出处 《中华行为医学与脑科学杂志》 CAS CSCD 北大核心 2019年第4期359-364,共6页 Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medicine and Brain Science
基金 国家自然科学基金项目(31660280).
关键词 面孔识别 整体加工 组合面孔效应 高动机情绪 事件相关电位 Face recognition Holistic processes Composite-face effect High-motivated affect Event-related potential
  • 相关文献

参考文献4

二级参考文献55

  • 1罗跃嘉,Raja Parasuraman.早期ERP效应与视觉注意空间等级的脑调节机制[J].心理学报,2001,33(5):385-389. 被引量:22
  • 2杨红升.自我面孔识别的独特性[J].心理科学,2005,28(6):1517-1519. 被引量:16
  • 3杨红升,黄希庭.自我信息加工的独特性:来自人名识别研究的证据[J].心理科学,2007,30(5):1127-1129. 被引量:10
  • 4Pessoa L. How do emotion utive control? Trends in and motivation direct exec- Cognitive Sciences, 2009, 13(4) : 160-166.
  • 5Gable P A, Harmon-Jones E. The motivational di- mensional model of affect: Implications for breadth of attention, memory, and cognitive categorization. Emo- tion and Cognition, 2010, 24: 322-337.
  • 6Tomkins S. Affects as the primary motivational system. In: Arnold M, (ed). Feelings and emotion. New York : Academic Press, 1970.
  • 7Izard C E, Dougherty F E, Bloxom B M, et al. The differential emotions scale: A method of measuring the meaning of subjective experience of discrete emotions. Naschville : Vanderblit University, 1974.
  • 8Bradley M M, Codispoti M, Cuthbert B N, et al. E- motion and motivation I: Defensive and appetitive re- actions in picture processing. Emotion, 2001, 1 (3): 276-298.
  • 9Walden K, Pornpattananangkul N, Curlee A, et al. Posterior versus frontal theta activity indexes approach motivation during affective autobiographical memories. Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2015, 15 : 132-144.
  • 10Mize K D, Jones N A. Infant physiological and behav- ioral responses to loss of maternal attention to a so- cial-rival. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 2012, 83(1): 16-23.

共引文献21

相关作者

内容加载中请稍等...

相关机构

内容加载中请稍等...

相关主题

内容加载中请稍等...

浏览历史

内容加载中请稍等...
;
使用帮助 返回顶部